A Grounded Theory of Intuition among Occupational Therapists in Mental Health Practice

By Chaffey, Lisa; Unsworth, Carolyn et al. | British Journal of Occupational Therapy, July 2010 | Go to article overview

A Grounded Theory of Intuition among Occupational Therapists in Mental Health Practice


Chaffey, Lisa, Unsworth, Carolyn, Fossey, Ellie, British Journal of Occupational Therapy


Introduction

Clinical reasoning is a complex notion concerned with the ways in which therapists think in practice (Mattingly and Fleming 1994). As Fleming and Mattingly (1994) observed, therapists may have difficulty in describing clinical reasoning when engaged in therapy. These authors proposed that difficulty in articulating actions is due to a lack of language. Another explanation may be that therapists' clinical reasoning is informed by intuition as well as analysis, and intuition is more difficult to articulate.

Intuition has been defined as 'a whole; immediate possession of knowledge; and knowledge independent of the linear reasoning process' (Rew 1986, p23). This concept has received little attention in the occupational therapy literature. This paper describes a study of occupational therapists' understanding and use of intuition in mental health practice and proposes a grounded theory of intuition drawn from the findings of this study.

Literature review

In order to understand how intuition may guide practice, it is necessary to understand how therapists think in action. In 1986, a team of researchers in the United States, including Mattingly and Fleming, began the Clinical Reasoning Study (Mattingly and Fleming 1994). This project was a 4-year study combining ethnographic and action research methodologies. From this study, these authors suggested that occupational therapists think in three tracks of reasoning: procedural, interactive and conditional reasoning. Procedural reasoning occurs during problem definition and intervention selection. Interactive reasoning occurs when building collaborative relationships with clients. Conditional reasoning is considering a client's whole condition within broader social and temporal contexts (Mattingly and Fleming 1994). Furthering our understanding of clinical reasoning theory, Unsworth (2004) and Boyt Schell and Schell (2008) have identified a range of environmental and personal factors that influence clinical reasoning. These include extrinsic factors such as workplace protocols and intrinsic factors such as beliefs, values, ethics and motivation.

Mattingly's and Fleming's (1994) study was conducted mostly with clinicians in physical areas of practice, however, with less attention being paid to the clinical reasoning of occupational therapists practising in mental health. Studies in this area have found that occupational therapists practising in mental health place emphasis on interacting with and understanding their clients to guide their clinical reasoning (Fossey 1996, Ward 2003).

The clinical reasoning of experts has also been explored. In the United Kingdom (UK), Roberts (1996) sought to describe the content and process of clinical reasoning in 38 occupational therapists, some practising in mental health. She noted that pattern recognition had a place in expert reasoning, which was likely to influence the speed of problem identification. Roberts' (1996) findings have been supported by further studies of expert reasoning in occupational therapy in physical dysfunction. In an American ethnographic study, Gibson et al (2000) suggested that experts' reasoning was often influenced by patterns in client presentations and that expert therapists were quicker to prioritise important information compared with novices. Unsworth (2001) reported similar findings in a mixed method study involving two novice and three expert occupational therapists in Australia. She reported that experts had a larger bank of clinical experiences from which to draw, generalising from their experiences twice as often as novices. These studies' findings are consistent with those of Dreyfus and Dreyfus (1986), who determined that experts have an intuitive understanding of a situation and the appropriate actions to take, supported by the exploration of Benner et al (1996) of intuition in expert nurses. However, an understanding of the role of intuition in occupational therapy practice is in its infancy. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

A Grounded Theory of Intuition among Occupational Therapists in Mental Health Practice
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.