Restrained Drinking and Cognitive Control among Adolescents

By Williams, Robert J.; Ricciardelli, Lina A. | Adolescence, Fall 1999 | Go to article overview

Restrained Drinking and Cognitive Control among Adolescents


Williams, Robert J., Ricciardelli, Lina A., Adolescence


ABSTRACT

The present study investigated restrained drinking and self-control in relation to alcohol consumption and problem drinking in a sample of adolescents. One hundred ninety-eight high school students (97 males and 101 females; mean age = 16.45 years) completed questionnaires that assessed levels of alcohol consumption, problem drinking, restrained drinking, and cognitive self-control. The findings were similar for males and females: higher levels of restrained drinking, as measured by cognitive emotional preoccupation (CEP), predicted higher levels of alcohol consumption and problem drinking. Moreover, it was high CEP in combination with a low score on a general measure of cognitive self-control that differentiated problem drinking from high levels of drinking. The results are consistent with previous research on young adults. The findings are discussed in relation to Baumeister and Heatherton's (1996) model of self-regulation failure.

The early onset, as well as the severity, of drinking problems among adolescents continues to be of concern. Levels of alcohol consumption in this age group are alarmingly high, and it has been reported that 35% develop at least one symptom of alcohol dependence/abuse by 19 years of age (Martin, Langenbucher, Kaczynski, & Chung, 1996; Lewinsohn, Rohde, & Seeley, 1996; Winters, Stinchfield, Fulkerson, & Henly, 1993).

Young people develop positive expectancies about the effects of alcohol even before they actually start drinking, primarily via imitation and modeling (Christiansen, Goldman, & Inn, 1982; Schafer & Leigh, 1996). These expectancies range from global satisfaction to more specific beliefs that alcohol use will result in assertiveness, pleasure, relaxation, and sexual enhancement (Brown, Christiansen, & Goldman, 1987).

The interactions between expectations of alcohol as a potent change agent and adolescents' self-concepts are crucial (Davey, 1990; Towber man & McDonald, 1993). Young adolescents are on the threshold of what has been called the "social press" of drinking (Brennan, Walfish, & Buchon, 1986), and admonitions about control must compete with the lure of enhancing one's reputation by taking risks (Hopkins & Emler, 1990; Keefe, 1994; Odgers, Houghton, & Douglas, 1996). Drinking to excess is one of these risk-taking behaviors.

Excessive drinking has many negative consequences. For example, it can interfere with scholastic performance. Indeed, the failure to meet role obligations (e.g., at home or school) characterizes "heavy and heedless drinking," which is the initial DSM-IV alcohol-related symptom to appear among adolescents (Martin et al., 1996).

Exerting control over alcohol consumption involves some degree of response conflict (Bensley, 1991). Unfortunately, a widespread method of control--labeled restraint--has been shown to be counterproductive. Restraint is typified by rigid control interspersed with bouts of bingeing (i.e., loss of control). The paradox of restraint is that trying harder to exert control actually predicts greater instability in subsequent behavior, rather than improvement in self-regulation (see Collins, 1993, for a review). In short, increased cognitive/emotional efforts amounting to preoccupation with, and worries about, control often increase the gulf between intention and behavior.

In adults, drinking restraint has been shown to predict binge drinking, problem drinking, and dependent styles of drinking (Collins & Lapp, 1992; Collins, Gollnisch, & Izzo, 1996; Connor, Williams, & Ricciardelli, in press). Since many adolescents develop an early form of alcohol dependence/abuse (Martin et al., 1996), the construct of restraint can prove useful in examining adolescent drinking behavior.

Two important aspects of restraint are cognitive emotional preoccupation (CEP) and cognitive behavioral control (OBO). The distinction between CEP, preoccupation with control, and CEO, actual attempts at control, not only enables the success of restraint to be measured, but also reveals the dynamic interplay between cognition and behavior. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Restrained Drinking and Cognitive Control among Adolescents
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.