Using Probability Experiments to Foster Discourse

By Edwards, Thomas G.; Hensien, Sarah M. | Teaching Children Mathematics, April 2000 | Go to article overview

Using Probability Experiments to Foster Discourse


Edwards, Thomas G., Hensien, Sarah M., Teaching Children Mathematics


Mathematics instructional programs in elementary school should include opportunities for students to understand and apply basic concepts related to chance and probability. The discussion draft of Principles and Standards for School Mathematics suggests that "students in grades 3-5 begin to actively consider the likelihood of events. At these levels, informal explorations involving probability are appropriate" (NCTM 1998, 181).

These informal investigations might take the form of probability experiments. Students have a natural curiosity about such experiments. Many are quick to speculate about the likely outcomes of the experiments, and most have hunches about them. Students' hunches may enable them to correctly identify the more likely of two outcomes (e.g., a coin is more likely to land on heads than a number cube is to land on a 5). The goal, however, should be to help students begin to quantify these notions, that is, to state the chance of a coin's landing on heads as "one out of two" and the chance of a number cube's landing on a 5 as "one out of six."

The probability experiments and discussions described in this article are possible avenues for students to pursue their mathematical hunches about chance. When students examine their hunches, the mathematical concepts embedded in the experiments will emerge naturally. Students should come away from these experiences with a sense that numbers can play a useful role in describing the chance that an event will occur, as well as an understanding of the nature of events that are equally likely to happen.

The Experiments

In the vignette that follows, the students are fifth graders who have had little or no previous experience studying probability. The teacher, Ms. Johnson, has organized the class in six groups of four, and the groups are working on three different probability experiments. Johnson hopes that these experiments will help her students--

* construct a concept of equally likely events,

* assign a theoretical probability to events, and

* relate the theoretical probability of an event to the observed relative frequency of that event during the experiment.

Figure 1 shows the directions for each of the experiments on which the students are working. All three of these experiments involve events that have equally likely outcomes: flipping a single coin; spinning an evenly divided spinner; and tossing a single number cube, or die.

Johnson has chosen to have the six groups repeat two of the experiments 25 times and one, 30 times to yield a data pool of 150 or 180 repetitions of each experiment. In each instance, the total number of repetitions is divisible by the number of possible outcomes for the experiment. The results will facilitate students' discussions about how many occurrences of each event should be expected.

Johnson has decided to use three experiments because she does not want her students to develop misconceptions, such as that "equally likely events must involve just two (or any other specific number of) possibilities." She also expects that by doing three different experiments, her students will have three different opportunities to compare the theoretical probability of an event with its observed relative frequency. In discussing the issue of equal likelihood, Johnson believes that the idea of quantification of chance will emerge naturally. That is, students will begin to think of and express the likelihood of these events in numerical terms as a result of their direct experience with the experiments.

The following vignette was compiled from our analysis of several similar lessons. The description of the whole-class discussions represents our best attempt to capture the spirit of the actual discourse that occurred in those classrooms.

The Discourse

The groups have collected and recorded data at each of the stations and have finished pooling their results for each experiment to obtain data for the whole class. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Using Probability Experiments to Foster Discourse
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.