CULTURE, LANGUAGE AND BEHAVIOR: Perception

By Russell, Charles G. | ETC.: A Review of General Semantics, Spring 2000 | Go to article overview

CULTURE, LANGUAGE AND BEHAVIOR: Perception


Russell, Charles G., ETC.: A Review of General Semantics


CHARLES G. RUSSELL [*]

Part I

When We Perceive, Do We "Discover" or "Conclude"?

WHEN SOMEONE perceives something, what happens? Your answer to this question can help you understand how you conceptualize perception. If you view perception as "paying attention to things," you probably equate perception with attending or searching for stimuli in the environment. If, however, you view perception as "making sense of the environment," you probably equate perception with an outcome or conclusion.

At least two basic and different views about perception exist. One focuses on attending and concentrates on surveying the environment. Another focuses on interpretation and concentrates on language and mental activity after initial contact with stimuli. Viewing perception as an attending process and as an outcome of attending can both serve us well.

We can consider both views helpful in understanding what happens when we perceive something. I consider attending the initiation of a process that ends with an outcome. Without attending, conclusions would in effect represent an outcome "without a start." Without outcomes, attending represents action without termination. Outcomes "without a start" and action without termination seem to me not very useful for understanding perception.

We Can View Perceptions as a Process

At this point you might simply expect an answer to the question "What is perception?" I cannot offer "the answer" to this question. I can only offer an answer to the question. Perception can represent a process that contributes to an outcome. When perception takes place, people attend to stimuli and formulate conclusions about them. We should probably neither think of perception as the start nor the finish of the process, but rather as the entire act.

This view of perception can suggest viewing it from different and not necessarily contradictory perspectives has merit. We can identify various aspects of the process and consider how they interact. Insofar as considering the perception process helps us understand what happens, perception becomes less mysterious and much more subject to control than random or unconscious activity.

Section one of this article includes a brief review of the attending aspects of perception, section two includes possible influences on attending, and section three includes possible relationships among attending, influences on perception and outcomes of perceiving.

Using a camera analogy can contribute to understanding the attending aspects of perception. For example, cameras loaded with film record stimuli within their range. In a very simplistic sense the camera represents a mechanical device that "attends to stimuli within its range." Of course we could dispute this simplistic view of cameras, but if we accept it for purposes of illustration, we can use the camera analogy to understand one view of human attending to stimuli within an environment.

The view that offers cameras and human perception as "capturing everything within their range" should perhaps receive additional consideration. Alleging that cameras and humans "capture everything within their range" can encourage overlooking what they do not "capture." Unfortunately, analogies in general, and this one in particular, can contribute to overlooking important aspects of things alleged "to be alike." For example, cameras lacking necessary features such as lens power and fast shutter speeds and loaded with black and white 100 speed film cannot record everything within their range. Black and white film will prevent cameras from recording color, slow film will prevent recording some poorly lighted stimuli, lens quality will determine detail recorded, etc. Cameras can only record stimuli they have the capacity to detect.

Cameras do not record everything in front of them. As mechanical devices, their physical nature and the film used determine what they detect and record. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

CULTURE, LANGUAGE AND BEHAVIOR: Perception
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.