Neuroscience and Early Childhood? A Dangerous Liaison

By Corrie, Loraine | Australian Journal of Early Childhood, June 2000 | Go to article overview

Neuroscience and Early Childhood? A Dangerous Liaison


Corrie, Loraine, Australian Journal of Early Childhood


Introduction

In asserting that neuroscience and early childhood is a `necessary partnership', Talay-Ongan joins the views of enthusiastic writers from the USA that are sweeping the field of education. Brain research is fascinating. It has the capacity to capture the imagination and thrill our desire for new discoveries with its use of cutting-edge technology. The media enjoy brain research, as it is embedded in hard-nosed scientific endeavour, and the computer-generated images make exciting copy. However, I urge early childhood professionals to avoid being charmed by the `looks good, feels good' factor. Neuroscience and early childhood appears to be a logical partnership, but we need to consider the research base before being seduced by persuasive rhetoric.

In answer to Talay-Ongan's claim that neuroscience will `enrich the field of early childhood', I argue that it is too soon to apply neuroscience to practice, because many studies are exploratory and offer few conclusive findings. In addition, prematurely coupling neuroscience with early childhood may be a dangerous liaison that could lead to an erosion of social justice for some groups of children. In particular, I suggest that the idea that `enriched environments' are important for children's brain development is not value neutral. Embedded in judgements about `enrichment' are moral commitments and ethical assumptions that need to be considered by early childhood professionals.

Background

Neuroscience as the panacea for educational ills is an appealing idea that has been easy to sell to the public (Le Doux, 1996). Governments welcome the `quick fix' and it is no coincidence that brain research and education connections have been supported by the United States Congress (Begley, 1996).

Talay-Ongan reflects the popular views of educators who are effusive in their support of brain research, for example, stating it is `indispensable' to `understand and apply this knowledge to our practice with young children'. However, it is important for early childhood professionals to examine the research base that substantiates any calls for change to practice, and we need to ask who is pushing the neuroscience/education partnership. Bruer (1998a) notes that educational futurists are transposing brain research to education, but neuroscientists have warned consistently against using their studies to guide practice. Fitzpatrick (a neuroscientist) said in a conversation with Jones (1995, p.24):

   Anything that people would say right now has a good chance of not being
   true two years from now because the understanding is so rudimentary and
   people are looking at things at such a simplistic level (my emphasis).

A partial picture of the research base may be conveyed when writers (such as Shore, 1997) simplify highly specialised scientific material for a broad audience, and caveats about the research findings may be easy to ignore. There have been many publications that show educators have ignored the neuroscientists' advice. Some authors talk about brain research as if conclusive evidence has been established; for example, Bimonte (1998, p. 16) asserts:

   The key to all learning is experience. It is experience that creates
   dendrites--the hair-like extensions that grow from neurons and create the
   neural pathways necessary for learning. The very best dendrites are grown
   from real world experience.

Bimonte does not cite evidence to support his view, and critics may say that currently such evidence does not exist. The lack of evidence has not hindered writers making many assumptions about the brain and scholastic achievements, and there has been a flurry of new publications to promote `brain based learning' (for example see Diamond & Hopson, 1998).

At times writers appear to confuse the fields of neuroscience and cognitive psychology, which is seen when `learning styles' are referred to in discussion of neuroscience. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Neuroscience and Early Childhood? A Dangerous Liaison
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.