The Impact of Metamemory on Reading Performance. (Language Teaching & Learning)

By Reza, Gholam; Nezhad, Haji Pour | Academic Exchange Quarterly, Fall 2001 | Go to article overview

The Impact of Metamemory on Reading Performance. (Language Teaching & Learning)


Reza, Gholam, Nezhad, Haji Pour, Academic Exchange Quarterly


Abstract

The purpose of this research was to determine 1) if a metamemory approach to reading instruction would lead to significantly different results from those of a skill-oriented approach, and 2) whether short-term memory (STM) span is an independent determining factor or it is modified by the reader's metamemory knowledge. Seventy-six college students were randomly selected and assigned to the two experimental and control groups. One group was taught through a metamemory approach and the other through a skill-oriented one. The treatment took about four months and a half, and a twelve-branch memory maze and The Nelson's Reading Comprehension Test were used as the STM span and the reading comprehension tests respectively. The findings of the ANOVA used lend support to the notion that a metamemory approach is superior to a skill-oriented one. They also indicate that STM span is not an independent predictor of reading comprehension performance. Moreover, the statistical evidence provided through the application of the Point Biserial Correlation to the pre-tests of reading comprehension and STM span indicate that there is no significant go-togetherness between STM span and performance on reading comprehension tests.

**********

The majority of the studies conducted on reading focus on the product of reading, that is, the decoding of words or the comprehension of texts rather than its process. However, a number of researchers have more recently focused their attention on the underlying aspect of reading, namely its process (e.g., Carpenter, 1987; Hasselhorn, 1991; Whitney, 1991). Metamemory, as one of the consequences of this shift, has gained a particular, and at the same time, controversial status. Originally, Flavell (1971) used the term metamemory to denote knowledge of memory. Following this, researchers started to conduct various studies on metamemory: Moynahan (1973) asked Ss about the relative difficulty of different tasks.

Brown (1978) studied how Ss monitored their own memory attempts. Brown & Smiley (1978) investigated students' use of strategies and found that active strategy users have superior recall to those students who simply re-read the text. Murphy and Schmitt (1987) found that strategies such as self-testing can be easily taught, and they hold promise of being useful in different situations. Gearg, Adrales & Klosterman (1990) found consistent correlation between metamemory and academic achievement. The variety of the areas where metamemory has been applied has been well manifested in Klatzky's (1984) classification of three general types of metamemory or awareness of memory: Conscious awareness of the mental processes in which one is engaged (e.g., rehearsal and organization); awareness of the contents that are stored in memory (the material one decides to encode); and awareness about memory as a human capacity. According to Klatzky (1984), a successful metamemory knowledge comprises all the three mentioned forms.

Hasselhorn (1992) maintains that metamemory establishes a substantive positive relationship between memory monitoring and memory performance. What this indicates is that memory performance is the focus of attention in metamemory studies. In other words, studies in this area aim at improving memory performance by resorting to certain types of intervention.

Now, if we assume, as other studies have done so, that metamemory influences memory performance, the question is what is meant by "memory". The first answer (model) to this apparently simple question was presented by Atkinson & Shiffrin (1971), who theorized that there are three memory stores -- a sensory register, a short-term and a long-term store, and who hypothesized that these memory stores are considered to be the permanent, built-in components of the information processing system. The idea of having fixed built-in memory stores gave rise to the public opinion that the individual's memory performance is not influenced by circumstances but by the individual's static memory span. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

The Impact of Metamemory on Reading Performance. (Language Teaching & Learning)
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.