The Attentional Blink Is Governed by a Temporary Loss of Control

By Kawahara, Jun-Ichiro; Kumada, Takatsune et al. | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, October 2006 | Go to article overview

The Attentional Blink Is Governed by a Temporary Loss of Control


Kawahara, Jun-Ichiro, Kumada, Takatsune, Di Lollo, Vincent, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


Identification of the second of two brief targets is impaired at intertarget lags of less than about 500 msec. We compared two accounts of this attentional blink (AB) by manipulating the number of digit distractors-and hence the lag-inserted among three letter targets in a rapid serial visual presentation stream of digit distractors. On the resource-depletion hypothesis, longer lags provide more time for processing the leading target, thus releasing resources for the trailing target. On the temporary-loss-of-control (TLC) hypothesis, intervening distractors disrupt the current attentional set, producing a trailing-target deficit. Identification accuracy for trailing targets was unimpaired not only at lag 1 (conventional lag 1 sparing) but also at later lags, if preceded by another target. The results supported the TLC hypothesis but not the resource-depletion hypothesis. We conclude that the AB is caused by a disruption in attentional set when a distractor is presented while the central executive is busy processing a leading target.

When two visual stimuli are presented in close temporal succession, identification is almost perfect for the first stimulus (T1) but substantially impaired for the second (T2). This second-target deficit, called the attentional blink (AB; Raymond, Shapiro, & Arnell, 1992), has been studied using a paradigm in which observers are required to identify two targets (e.g., letters) inserted in a stream of distractors (e.g., digits) displayed in rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP). All items in the RSVP stream are presented in the same spatial location at a rate of approximately 10 items/sec. The temporal lag between the two targets is manipulated by varying the number of intervening distractors. The second-target deficit is most pronounced at short intertarget lags, with performance improving progressively as the lag is increased, reaching an asymptote at about 500 msec.

Theoretical accounts of the AB have focused on the processing of T1 as the primary source of the second-target deficit. On these accounts (e.g., Chun & Potter, 1995; Jolicoeur & Dell' Acqua, 1998; Shapiro, Raymond, & Arnell, 1994), the requirement to process T1 is said to delay the allocation of attentional resources to T2 for several hundred milliseconds. As a result, if T2 is presented shortly after T1, its processing is delayed, and it becomes vulnerable to overwriting by subsequent stimuli (see, e.g., Giesbrecht & Di Lollo, 1998). At longer lags, resources initially deployed to T1 become available for T2 as T1 processing is nearing completion, and the AB is no longer in evidence. In brief, current theoretical accounts of the AB hold to the idea of resource depletion as the principal factor in the second-target deficit.

Findings inconsistent with a resource-depletion account have been reported by Di Lollo, Kawahara, Ghorashi, and Enns (2005) in a study in which observers were required to identify three consecutive target letters inserted in an RSVP stream of digit distractors. According to the resource-depletion hypothesis, identification accuracy should be highest for the leading target and decline progressively for each successive target as resources become more depleted. Instead, no progressive decrement (i.e., no AB deficit) was in evidence over the successive targets: The third target (T3) was identified as accurately as the first. A second, and even more revealing finding was that identification of T3 was substantially impaired-as in the conventional AB deficit-when the middle letter in the three-target string was replaced with a digit. This is all the more remarkable because the observers were required to report only two targets rather than three targets.

An account of these findings has been proposed by Di Lollo et al. (2005) in terms of a temporary loss of control (TLC) over the prevailing attentional set. The TLC account is based on two assumptions. First, that at the outset of the RSVP stream, the observer adopts an attentional set aimed at accepting targets and rejecting distractors. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

The Attentional Blink Is Governed by a Temporary Loss of Control
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.