Evidence for Separate Representations for Action and Location in Implicit Motor Sequencing

By Witt, Jessica K.; Willingham, Daniel T. | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, October 2006 | Go to article overview

Evidence for Separate Representations for Action and Location in Implicit Motor Sequencing


Witt, Jessica K., Willingham, Daniel T., Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


We examined sequential learning of actions in an experiment in which four different actions (push, twist, pinch, switch) were placed at four horizontal locations. At transfer, participants responded to a sequence that required performing the same sequence of actions at different locations and to a different sequence of actions at the same sequence of locations. Participants with explicit knowledge demonstrated only learning the sequence of response locations. However, participants with implicit knowledge learned the sequence of actions just as well as the sequence of locations, and performance on individual sequences was just as good as performance when both sequences were presented. These results demonstrated that two types of sequences, one of actions and another of response locations, can be learned simultaneously, suggesting that parallel representations are involved in implicit motor skill acquisition.

Skilled actions in everyday life require sequential knowledge in order to prepare responses in advance. Motor skill learning has often been explored in the lab using the serial response time (SRT) paradigm (Nissen & Bullemer, 1987). In this task, participants respond to stimuli that, unbeknownst to the participants, sometimes appear in a repeating sequence and other times appear randomly. Learning is assessed in two ways. One measure is the time it takes to respond to the sequenced stimuli relative to random stimuli; this measure is considered implicit because it is based on performance and is not associated with awareness of the sequence. The second measure is an interview, or recognition or recall measure. These measures are considered explicit because participants know that their memory is being tapped, and performance is necessarily associated with awareness of die material remembered. Interestingly, response times (RTs) often decrease even though the participant is unaware of the sequence (Willingham, Nissen, & Bullemer, 1989). Some researchers claim that the two types of learning represent the workings of different memory systems (e.g., Squire & ZolaMorgan, 1991), which raises the possibility that the two systems rely on different types of representations.

Most investigations on the nature of the representation for motor skill acquisition have used buttonpressing as the response, and it is well established that people are capable of learning sequences of response locations implicitly (e.g., Nattkemper & Prinz, 1997; Willingham, Wells, Farrell, & Stemwedel, 2000; but see Remillard, 2003). This task provides a useful way to study skills, such as piano playing and typing, for which the actions (keypresses) vary little and responses are differentiated by spatial location. However, other types of motor skills require learning a sequence of different actions. For example, dancers and martial artists learn sequences of different kinds of movements; however, this learning is usually explicit. To our knowledge, no research has investigated whether action sequences can be learned implicitly. By action sequence, we mean a sequence in which the responses can be differentiated by the action required to make the response. In the typical implicit SRT tasks, participants respond with the same action throughout the sequence (keypresses), so the sequence cannot be learned as a sequence of distinct actions and, rather, is typically learned as a sequence of response locations.

To address whether action sequences can be learned implicitly, we used a modified SRT task. Participants responded to spatial stimuli on a response board with four manipulanda, each requiring a unique action. At transfer, the participants responded on a different board in which the manipulanda were placed at different spatial locations. They responded to a sequence that required performing the same sequence of actions at different locations and a sequence that required performing the same sequence of response locations but with a different sequence of actions. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Evidence for Separate Representations for Action and Location in Implicit Motor Sequencing
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.