WHAT CHANGES IN COGNITIVE THERAPY? THE ROLE OF TACIT KNOWLEDGE STRUCTURES: Theoretical Considerations-New Ideas

By Dowd, E. Thomas | Journal of Evidence-Based Psychotherapies, September 2006 | Go to article overview

WHAT CHANGES IN COGNITIVE THERAPY? THE ROLE OF TACIT KNOWLEDGE STRUCTURES: Theoretical Considerations-New Ideas


Dowd, E. Thomas, Journal of Evidence-Based Psychotherapies


Abstract

Rarely has knowledge within cognitive psychology influenced the development and practice of cognitive behavior therapy. This article explores the integration of the contributions of the cognitive psychology areas of implicit learning, tacit knowledge structures, and encoding processes with producing or retarding change in cognitive behavior therapy. Differences are discussed between understanding something and knowing something. Implications for the practice of cognitive therapy, indeed all psychotherapy are discussed. A clinical example is provided.

Key words: tacit knowledge, implicit learning, cognitive encoding, cognitive therapy

WHAT CHANGES IN COGNITIVE THERAPY? THE ROLE OF TACIT KNOWLEDGE STRUCTURES

The fact that cognitive psychotherapy results in psychological and behavior change is undeniable - many studies have demonstrated that to be true. On the other hand, cognitive psychotherapy has not been shown to be consistently more effective than alternative therapies. In a recent short revie w of the comparative outcome literature, Dowd (2004a) concluded that the evidence for the greater efficacy of cognitive therapy is mixed, especially when compared with specific alternative treatments rather than with placebo or generic and supportive types of therapy. There is some evidence, however, that cognitive therapy may be especially useful in preventing relapse or with certain specific problems such as panic disorder. Whisman (1999) argued that the evidence is equivocal regarding whether cognitive change precedes or follows symptom change.

Why is this happening? Why, despite our best efforts, can we not develop cognitive therapy procedures that are more effective that other targeted treatments? Why can we not even agree whether cognitive changes cause behavioral changes or results from behavior change (e.g. Whisman, 1993) or whether cognitive change precedes or follows cognitive change (Whisman, 1999)? Why, as Imber and his colleagues (Imber et al, 1990) discovered, did neither cognitive therapy nor other therapies, produce the specific results that were predicted by their theoretical orientations? Is cognitive therapy even necessary at all? Jacobson and his colleagues (1996) argued it is not necessary and that behavioral activation is enough. Likewise, Feeney, Hembree, and Zoellner (2003) argued that the research literature has not demonstrated that adding additional interventions (such as cognitive therapy) to exposure therapy produced better outcomes than exposure alone.

These questions cannot be answered in one article but a partial answer is possible that might account for some of these findings. To do so I suggest that the fundamental conceptual model of cognitive behavior therapy be expanded to include changes in implicit knowledge structures as well as changes in explicit knowledge. More specifically I propose an integration of certain features of tacit or implicit learning in cognitive psychology with the theory of cognitive change. As David and Szentagotai (2005) have pointed out, cognitive psychology and cognitive behavioral psychotherapy developed independently and rarely has the former influenced the latter. This article is an attempt to do that in a limited area of knowledge.

Years ago, Hobbs (1962) suggested - contrary to the wisdom of that day - that cognitive change (insight) is the result of behavior change, not the cause of it. Clients, he said, changed their behavior and then reflected cognitively about what that new behavior meant and its implications. Later, and in the same vein, Bandura (1977) said, "On one hand the mechanisms by which human behavior is acquired and regulated are increasingly formulated in terms of cognitive processes. On the other hand, it is performance-based procedures that are proving to be the most powerful for effecting psychological changes" (p. 191). Or, in more popular terms, "It is easier to act your way into a new way of thinking than to think your way into a new way of acting. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

WHAT CHANGES IN COGNITIVE THERAPY? THE ROLE OF TACIT KNOWLEDGE STRUCTURES: Theoretical Considerations-New Ideas
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

    Already a member? Log in now.