Affective Guidance in the Iowa Gambling Task

By Wagar, Brandon M.; Dixon, Mike | Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, December 2006 | Go to article overview

Affective Guidance in the Iowa Gambling Task


Wagar, Brandon M., Dixon, Mike, Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience


It has been suggested that affective states can guide higher level cognitive processes and that such affective guidance may be particularly important when real-life decisions are made under uncertainty. We ask whether affect guides decisions in a laboratory task that models real-life decisions under uncertainty. In the Iowa gambling task (IGT), participants search for monetary payoffs in an uncertain environment Recent evidence against an affective guidance interpretation of the IGT indicates a need to set a standard for what counts as evidence of affective guidance. We present a novel analysis of IGT, and our results show that participants' galvanic skin response (GSR) reflects an affective process that precedes and guides cognition. Specifically, prior to participants' knowledge of the optimal strategy, their GSRs are significantly higher when they are about to select from a bad deck, relative to a good deck, and this difference in GSR is correlated with a behavioral preference for the good deck.

The question of how much conscious control we have over our decisions is one of the most interesting and important questions of psychological research. Traditional thinking in cognitive psychology has assumed that complex decision making does not rely on, and is not necessarily influenced by, affective processes. However, recent research in psychology, behavioral economics, and neuroscience (Cohen & Blum, 2002; Kahneman, 2003) suggests that affective processes play an important role in many decision-making processes. Specifically, it has been suggested that affective processes precede and guide cognition (Zajonc, 1980) and that affective guidance may be particularly important when real-life decisions are made in an uncertain environment (Damasio, 1994). Affect, as it is understood here, is distinct from emotion. Affect can be understood in relation to the idea of core affect (Russell, 2003). At the heart of emotion, mood, and other emotionally charged events are conscious states experienced as simply feeling good or bad. These states, called core affect, can influence perception, cognition, and behavior even though people have no direct access to these causal connections. The central question we ask is whether affect guides decisions in a laboratory task that models real-life decisions in an uncertain environment.

In the Iowa gambling task (IGT; Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, 1994; Bechara, Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1997), participants search for monetary payoffs in an uncertain environment. Individuals are given a $2,000 loan of play money and four decks of cards and are instructed to make a series of card selections from any of the four decks, until they are told to stop. The participants are instructed to play so that they maximize profit on the loan. Turning each card carries an immediate reward (which is large for Decks A and B and small for Decks C and D). In addition, after turning some cards, the participants receive a punishment (which is large for Decks A and B and small for Decks C and D). Decks A and B are equivalent in terms of overall net loss, but the punishment is more frequent and of smaller magnitude for Deck A than for Deck B. Decks C and D are equivalent in terms of overall net gain, but the punishment is more frequent and of smaller magnitude for Deck C than for Deck D. The resulting combination of rewards and punishments results in an optimal strategy, so that playing from Decks A or B (bad decks) results in an overall loss, whereas playing from Decks C or D (good decks) results in an overall profit. The participants are told that they are free to switch from any deck to another at any time and as often as they wish. However, the participants are not told how many card selections must be made and are blind to the punishment schedules.

Typically, the participants adopt the optimal strategy of picking predominantly from good decks and avoiding bad decks. In fact, the participants initially start to show a physiological preference by exhibiting higher galvanic skin responses (GSRs) when they are about to select a card from a bad deck than when they are about to select a card from a good deck, even though their subjective reports fail to reveal conscious knowledge of the optimal strategy. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Affective Guidance in the Iowa Gambling Task
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

    Already a member? Log in now.