Gender Differences in Object Location Memory: A Meta-Analysis

By Voyer, Daniel; Postma, Albert et al. | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, February 2007 | Go to article overview

Gender Differences in Object Location Memory: A Meta-Analysis


Voyer, Daniel, Postma, Albert, Brake, Brandy, Imperato-McGinley, Julianne, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


The goal of the present study was to quantify the magnitude of gender differences in object location memory tasks. A total of 123 effect sizes (d) drawn from 36 studies were included in a meta-analysis using a hierarchical approach. Object identity memory (37 effect sizes) and object location memory (86 effect sizes) tasks were analyzed separately. Object identity memory task showed significant gender differences that were homogeneous and in favor of women. For the object location memory tasks, effect sizes had to be partitioned by age (younger than 13, between 13 and 18, older than 18), object type (common, uncommon, gender neutral, geometric, masculine, feminine), scoring method (accuracy, time, distance), and type of measure (recall, recognition) to achieve homogeneity. Significant gender differences in favor of females were obtained in all clusters above the age of 13, with the exception of feminine, uncommon, and gender-neutral objects. Masculine objects and measures of distance produced significant effects in favor of males. Implications of these results for future work and for theoretical interpretations are discussed.

When considering the question of differences in cognitive abilities between men and women, there are specific areas that come to mind quite readily. For a number of years, researchers have been aware that the domains of spatial and mathematical abilities seem especially relevant to the question of cognitive gender differences since they yield marked differences in favor of males (Benbow, 1988; Hedges &Nowell, 1995; Hyde, Fennema, & Lamon, 1990; Linn & Petersen, 1985; Voyer, Voyer, & Bryden, 1995). Although verbal abilities were long believed to be in favor of women (see, e.g., Maccoby & Jacklin, 1974), more recent reviews suggest that they are either weakly in favor of women, not significant at all, or even in favor of men, depending on the task considered (Hedges & Nowell, 1995; Hyde & Linn, 1988). At the time Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) wrote their landmark book, many believed that spatial, mathematical, and verbal abilities were the only areas where cognitive gender differences occurred.

More recent work, however, suggests that an area of cognitive abilities that was not mentioned by Maccoby and Jacklin (1974) offers potential for emerging gender differences. Specifically, in the early 1990s, a number of researchers reported gender differences in favor of females in tasks requiring memory for the location of objects (see, e.g., Crook, Youngjohn, & Larrabee, 1990; Sharps, WeIton, & Price, 1993; Silverman & EaIs, 1992). In what is probably the most cited of these studies, Silverman and EaIs (1992) developed a task that they claimed reflected a yet unexplored aspect of spatial abilities, namely spatial memory. In this task, participants were presented with a randomly organized array of line drawings on a single piece of paper. Participants studied this array for 1 min, followed by a new piece of paper on which they were required to identify which objects were old (found in the study array) and which ones had been added. This task is labeled as the objects identity memory task hereafter, and it was presented by Silverman and EaIs as a measure of memory that is independent of location. Finally, a third array was presented in which some of the objects that had been found in the study array had been moved, whereas others remained where they were (unmoved). Participants were required to identify the moved and unmoved objects. This task is labeled as the conventional object location memory task hereafter, and it was presented as a measure of object location memory by Silverman and Eals. The authors found that women performed better than men in both tasks. Since then, the results obtained in this initial study (Study 1 of the Silverman & Eals, 1992, article) have been replicated several times by various researchers with the conventional object location memory task. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Sign up now for a free, 1-day trial and receive full access to:

  • Questia's entire collection
  • Automatic bibliography creation
  • More helpful research tools like notes, citations, and highlights
  • A full archive of books and articles related to this one
  • Ad-free environment

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Gender Differences in Object Location Memory: A Meta-Analysis
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Sign up now to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    For full access in an ad-free environment, sign up now for a FREE, 1-day trial.

    Already a member? Log in now.