Self-Awareness and Cognitive Style: Relationships among Adaption-Innovation, Self-Monitoring, and Self-Consciousness

By Hutchinson, Lynda R.; Skinner, Nicholas F. | Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal, April 20, 2007 | Go to article overview

Self-Awareness and Cognitive Style: Relationships among Adaption-Innovation, Self-Monitoring, and Self-Consciousness


Hutchinson, Lynda R., Skinner, Nicholas F., Social Behavior and Personality: an international journal


Adaption-innovation is a construct of preferred problem-solving style; adaptors work best within clear guidelines and prefer to "do things better", whereas innovators bridle at structure and prefer to "do things differently". Adaption-innovation bears considerable putative similarity to self-monitoring and self-consciousness. In this study the relationships among these constructs were explored using the responses of 55 undergraduate students (48 females, 7 males) on the Kirton Adaption-Innovation Inventory (KAI; Kirton, 1976), the Self-Monitoring Scale (Snyder & Gangestad, 1986) and the Self-Consciousness Scale (Fenigstein, Scheier, & Buss, 1975). Higher adaption-innovation scores were significantly and positively associated with higher self-monitoring scores and significantly and negatively associated with social anxiety scores. In addition, multiple regression analyses indicated that the facets of self-consciousness as well as self-monitoring significantly predicted adaption-innovation. The implications of examining cognitive style in relation to interpersonal attributes are discussed.

Keywords: self-awareness, cognitive style, adaption-innovation, self-monitoring, problem solving, self-consciousness.

Throughout the psychology literature, researchers (e.g., Paulhus & Williams, 2002; Skinner & Drake, 2003) have linked a host of personality constructs (e.g., Machiavellianism, extraversion, openness to experience) to individuals' social behaviors and characteristics (e.g., altruism, aggression). More recent research has focused on bridging the gap between personality and cognition by linking theories of cognitive style to aspects of personality or self-hood (Sternberg & Grigorenko, 1997). In the present study the way in which the cognitive style construct of adaption-innovation is related to the personality constructs of self-monitoring and self-consciousness - which are described below - was examined.

ADAPTION-INNOVATION

According to Kirton (1976, 2000), adaption-innovation is a bipolar construct of cognitive style that identifies an individual's preferred approach to problem solving. "Adaptors" choose to work within existing guidelines or paradigms to achieve improved solutions, whereas "innovators" feel constrained by rules and opt to operate "outside the box" so as to solve problems differently. Researchers (e.g., Goldsmith & Matherly, 1987; Skinner, 1996) have found that adaptioninnovation is related to motivation for uniqueness, perceived creativity, and self-esteem. Despite these findings, it is not known if adaption-innovation is associated with self-monitoring and self-consciousness.

SELF-MONITORING

Self-monitoring refers to individuals' ability to regulate their behavior to meet the demands of social situations (Snyder, 1987). High self-monitors readily alter or adapt their behavior as necessary from one social context to the next; in contrast, low self-monitors exhibit what they regard as their true selves, and judge their behavior to be consistent across all social situations. The characteristics associated with self-monitoring resemble those associated with adaption-innovation. Specifically, (a) adaptors' consistent adherence to rules and guidelines appears similar to low self-monitoring (i.e., behaving consistently from one social situation to the next) and (b) innovators' preference for employing "different" solutions appears more characteristic of high self-monitoring (i.e., behaving differently from one social context to the next). In fact, Skinner and Perlini (1989) reported that (a) innovators scored significantly higher on selfmonitoring than did adaptors, and (b) high self-monitors were significantly more innovative than were low self-monitors. The present investigation was based in part on the work of Skinner and Perlini (1989); in addition, it extends the literature by examining how adaption-innovation and self-monitoring are related to self-consciousness. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Self-Awareness and Cognitive Style: Relationships among Adaption-Innovation, Self-Monitoring, and Self-Consciousness
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.