Cognitive Function of Centenarians and Its Influence on Longevity

By Gondo, Yasuyuki; Poon, Leonard W. | Annual Review of Gerontology & Geriatrics, January 1, 2007 | Go to article overview

Cognitive Function of Centenarians and Its Influence on Longevity


Gondo, Yasuyuki, Poon, Leonard W., Annual Review of Gerontology & Geriatrics


High cognitive function has been asserted to be an important contributor to longevity in general and especially in extreme longevity. The goals of this chapter are to (a) examine evidence for this assertion in normal and pathological aging, (b) identify confounding and not well understood issues, and (c) suggest future research directions relating to causal factors of cognition on longevity.

REVIEW OF SUPPORTIVE EVIDENCE

Impact Among the General Population

The Terman Cohort Study, which examined the life course of intellectually gifted individuals, addressed the relationship between individual differences in cognitive function and mortality (Friedman & Markey 2003). At the beginning of the study (1921), researchers recruited 1,528 younger people with IQs of 135 and higher. These individuals were followed 7 decades until 1991. Total mortality rates of these gifted individuals were lower than those of the general U.S. population born at the same time.

Similarly, a recent series of studies that utilized intelligence test data collected more than 50 years ago reported positive relationships between childhood intelligence and mortality in middle and very old age (Batty, Deary, & Macintyre, 2006; Deary, Bastin, et al., 2006; Hart et al., 2005; Shenkin, Starr, & Deary, 2004; Whalley & Deary, 2001; Whalley et al, 2000). Data came from the Scottish Mental Survey of 11-year-old children performed in 1932. The researchers linked these test scores with public morbidity and mortality data and found that a higher test score in childhood was associated with longer survival.

A study of nuns also addressed the relationship between individual differences in cognitive function and mortality (Snowdon, Greiner, Kemper, & Nanayakkara, 1999). The researchers analyzed linguistic ability from the participants' autobiographies written in their younger ages (18-32 years old). Two scores of linguistic ability-grammatical complexity and idea densitywere analyzed to examine the association between cognitive performances and mortality The results showed that idea density had a negative association with the risk of mortality Individuals with performance in the lowest quartile had a 7-year shorter life span than the higher performance group.

Bosworth and Siegler (2002) reviewed nine studies that evaluated the relationship between terminal decline of cognitive function and death. Although they did not find a consistent relationship, they did find that the level of cognitive function is predictive of mortality Recent statistically sophisticated analyses with representative-sample longitudinal studies reported the same pattern (Ghisletta, McArdle, & Lindenberger, 2006; Rabbitt, Lunn, & Wong, 2006). What causes the association between cognitive ability and mortality in elders? Medical condition and vitality decline are strong candidates. Hassing and colleagues (2002) followed 446 participants aged 80 to 98 for 6 years and found that participants with a shorter remaining life span showed lower cognitive ability in the baseline survey However, this association was attenuated after medical conditions such as stroke and cardiovascular disease were considered. The lower cognitive ability associated with death may be triggered by a decline of physical function or vitality.

Impact Among Centenarians

Six centenarian studies addressed the impact of cognition on longevity (Gondo et al., 2006; Hagberg, 2006; Kliegel & Sliwinski, 2004; Poon et al., 2000; Samuelsson et al., 1997; Shimizu, Hirose, Arai, Gondo, & Wakida, 2001). The French centenarian study surveyed 800 centenarians and followed them for 8 years until 99% had died. Cognitive impairment measured by the short portable mental status questionnaire, a test similar to the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE), significantly predicted survivorship as well as did residential condition, health status, activities of daily living (ADL), and independent activities of daily living (IADL). …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Cognitive Function of Centenarians and Its Influence on Longevity
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.