Teaching Personal Problem Solving to Adults

By Brammer, Lawrence | Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, January 1, 1990 | Go to article overview

Teaching Personal Problem Solving to Adults


Brammer, Lawrence, Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy


Solving personal problems is a basic life skill. The goal is to achieve problemsolving competence in applying a variety of models, three of which are trial and error/reinforcement, linear/rational, and intuitive/creative. The focus in this article is on the intuitive creative model that deals with novel problematic situations. Conditions of readiness for solving problems with any model are accurate problem awareness and appraisal of the problem, an attitude of meeting a challenge, selfefficacy or confidence in one's ability to solve problems, and an appropriate problem-solving set. While much research has been devoted to studying children and college youth in laboratory situations doing essentially thinking types of tasks, little research has been done with middle-aged and older adults. More research is needed on how adults learn problem-solving skills, and how they might develop greater problem-solving competence.

INTRODUCTION

Problem solving is a basic life skill, but few people are aware of their style or have a varied repertoire of personal problem-solving strategies. The purpose of this article is to survey the issues and methods for teaching selected problem-solving skills to adults. The goal for such instruction is to develop problem-solving effectiveness (Heppner & Krauskopf, 1987). This means that adults could become aware of their basic style of solving problems in everyday living, and that they would be able to choose the most appropriate strategy from a menu of problem-solving approaches.

The basic assumption underlying the discussion to follow is that personal problem solving can be categorized into distinct styles, and that these personal styles can be subdivided into teachable skills. The illustrative teaching strategy is a microskills training workshop (Ivey & Authier, 1978). This cognitive/behavioral approach involves learning a set of basic skills which are then assembled into a functional problem-solving process. So much of the literature on personal problem solving relates to learning such skills in a counseling format (Dixon & Glover, 1984; Haley, 1987; Heppner, 1978; Heppner, Reeder, & Larson, 1983; Hosford & DeVisser, 1976), but very little attention is devoted to learning personal problem solving with adult group methods.

There is disagreement in the literature on the definition and scope of personal problem solving. In this article, problem solving is considered one of many coping skills that have affective as well as cognitive components. Textbook discussions typically include problem solving as a component of cognition. Mayer (1983) considers thinking, problem solving, and cognition as interchangeable terms. Thinking is what a person does when he or she solves a problem. According to Mayer, a problem must have some givens (such as conditions and information), goals (end states desired), and obstacles (processes to reach the goal are not obvious). There appears to be general agreement among cognitive psychologists, however, that problem solving involves some rational steps that take the person toward a goal when that goal is uncertain. When reaching beyond the narrow range of linear and rational models that evolve from laboratory studies, the real-life problem-solving process becomes a "complex, dynamic, highly interactive, and intermittent process" (Heppner &Krauskopf, 1987, p. 375). D'Zurilla and Nezu(1982) state that problem solving is a process people use to cope with daily problematic situations. This means that the person is faced with obstacles or decisions for which no effective solutions or outcomes are apparent.

Following is a brief presentation of three common problem-solving process models and their component skills. The focus will be on intuitive problem solving, sometimes described as creative problem solving. Space does not allow detailed discussion of trial and error or linear and systems models; furthermore, the linear model is covered in detail in another article in this issue. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Teaching Personal Problem Solving to Adults
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.