The Case for Implicit Category Learning

By Smith, Edward E. | Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, March 2008 | Go to article overview

The Case for Implicit Category Learning


Smith, Edward E., Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience


This article evaluates the evidence regarding the claim that people can learn a novel category implicitly-that is, by an implicit memory system that is qualitatively different from an explicit system. The evidence that is considered is based on the prototype extraction task, in which participants are first exposed to a set of category exemplars under incidental learning instructions and are then required to categorize novel test items. Knowlton and Squire (1993) first reported that memory-impaired patients performed normally on the prototype extraction task while being impaired on a comparable recognition task. Several studies have replicated these results, but other articles have criticized the evidence for implicit category learning on both methodological and theoretical grounds. In this article, we consider five of these criticisms-for example, that the normal performance of the patients is due to intact working memory mechanisms (see, e.g., Palmeri & Flannery, 1999) or to the lesser cognitive demands of prototype extraction rather than recognition (e.g., Nosofsky & Zaki, 1998). For each of the five criticisms, we offer counterevidence that supports implicit category learning.

Psychologists and neuroscientists began to recognize the existence of an implicit memory system during the 1960s and early 1970s when they discovered that a medial temporal lobe (MTL) amnesic, H.M., could learn a new perceptual motor skill as readily as neurologically intact participants. Soon after, researchers discovered that MTL patients showed intact perceptual priming, which indicated that implicit memory is not confined to motor skills (see, e.g., Schacter, 1987, and Squire, 2004, for reviews of these early developments). These findings led to a characterization of implicit memory as a system that can use past experience without intention or awareness and that does not depend on MTL.

In 1993, Knowlton and Squire published the first article claiming to demonstrate that memory-impaired patients had an intact ability to learn a novel category. This is known as implicit category learning. These findings were important because they demonstrated a kind of implicit learning that extracts common features and supports generalizations to novel items, which seemed to go beyond perceptual priming or other known forms of implicit learning. However, in recent years, the evidence for implicit category learning has come under sharp criticism, primarily from researchers who have focused on behavioral and mathematical analyses of category learning (e.g., Nosofsky & Zaki, 1998; Zaki & Nosofsky, 2001 ). The purpose of this article is to review and evaluate the evidence for and against postulating an implicit category-learning system, particularly in light of recent findings on this topic (e.g., Bozoki, Grossman, & Smith, 2006; Reber, Gitelman, Parrish, & Mesulam, 2003). The argument will be made that these recent articles provide new evidence that (1) people can indeed learn a category implicitly, (2) patients with compromised MTL function learn roughly as well as normals, and (3) the learning involved may be a kind of perceptual fluency.

Initial Evidence for Implicit Category Learning

In their seminal study, Knowlton and Squire (1993) used a prototype extraction task. Both memory-impaired patients and normal controls were presented a series of 40 dot patterns (see Figure IA for examples). All of the patterns had been created by starting with a prototype pattern and then transforming it to some degree by moving some proportion of the dots (after Posner & Keele, 1968). During training, nothing was mentioned about a category, since both patients and controls were instructed simply to point to the center dot in each pattern. After training, all participants were informed that the patterns they had just seen "all belonged to a single category" and that they now had to determine which of a sequence of test patterns also belonged to that category. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

The Case for Implicit Category Learning
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.