Omitting Race: Politically Correct or Good Crime Reporting?

By Lehrman, Sally | The Quill, May 2008 | Go to article overview

Omitting Race: Politically Correct or Good Crime Reporting?


Lehrman, Sally, The Quill


Two frightening killers were on the loose, and the Sacramento Bee's readers wanted to protect themselves. They wanted more than descriptions of the attackers' clothing at the time of the murders. They wanted to know the criminals' race. The Bee, they accused editors, had allowed outdated policies to endanger public safety.

Challenged by readers and by bloggers who don't adhere to journalistic conventions, many editors have been thinking about loosening their rules for identifying race in crime stories.

In general, news outlets have avoided racial and ethnic identifiers unless they were important to the case, or, perhaps, if victims' descriptions were very detailed. They'd apply a test: Was the racial information useful to people in the community who might know the attacker or want to avoid harm themselves? Or was it so general that it only merely contributed to stereotypes about one group or another?

While many readers writing to the Bee and other outlets have attacked such reasoning as "political correctness" run amok, it's actually plain good sense. There's good reason to question the accuracy of most racial and ethnic identifiers, social science and legal experts have found, especially in crime situations. Why include anything that is vague and doesn't add accurate details to a story?

First, there's the fuzziness of the very description. The skin color of people who are black, white, Asian or Hispanic varies greatly and can overlap. Family history, geography, even the amount of sun that an area normally gets can make a big difference. The skin of a "Hispanic" man, for instance, could be anywhere from rich black to creamy white.

We have a long history of mixed offspring in this country, and as mixed race families grow in number, simple categories organized by skin color, hair texture and eye shape are less and less useful. In one crime reported in the Los Angeles Times in 2001, witnesses described a single robber variously as white, African American, Puerto Rican, Brazilian and Middle Eastern.

Consider, too, how "race" affects our thinking, especially in a situation like a crime. More often than any other type of error, wrongful convictions of innocent people can be traced back to mistaken identification. Memory is delicate, and especially so when it comes to emotional situations, as well as cross-racial descriptions. In one classic study testing racial prejudice, participants who witnessed an event with one black man and one white man were more likely to report that a black person held a knife even when it was the white man.

More recent implicit association tests have found that overall, white people more quickly "see" an object in a black man's hand as a weapon, even if it is a tool or a can of Coke. In a review of such research in the journal Psychology, Public Policy and Law, experimental psychologists Ralph Norman Haber and Lyn Haber concluded that eyewitnesses sometimes remember events more in accordance with their own beliefs and expectations than with whatever actually happened. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Omitting Race: Politically Correct or Good Crime Reporting?
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.