Perceptual Adaptation to Facial Asymmetries

By Rhodes, Gillian; Louw, Kim et al. | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, June 2009 | Go to article overview

Perceptual Adaptation to Facial Asymmetries


Rhodes, Gillian, Louw, Kim, Evangelista, Emma, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


Humans and other animals are highly sensitive to deviations from bilateral symmetry and prefer symmetric mates. Fluctuating asymmetries (FAs) are random deviations from perfect symmetry that can result from developmental instability. Human perceptions of facial asymmetry are driven by FAs and insensitive to directional asymmetries (DAs), which have a consistent direction of bias (e.g., left side always larger) across the population and are unrelated to developmental stability. We hypothesized that perceptual adaptation may filter out DAs and provide a proximate mechanism for this perceptual focus on FAs. We created a small population of faces with DAs by applying a unilateral distortion to the same side of each face. After 5 min of adaptation, (new) faces with these DAs looked less asymmetric and the most symmetric-looking distortion shifted toward the adapting asymmetry level. Parallel changes occurred for attractiveness. We suggest that perceptual adaptation may provide the proximate mechanism for an evolutionarily adaptive focus on FAs.

Humans and many other animals are highly sensitive to deviations from bilateral symmetry, and they prefer symmetric mates (Møller & Swaddle, 1997; Rhodes, 2006; Rhodes & Simmons, 2007). This preference may be an evolutionary adaptation for finding high-quality mates, given the link between developmental instability and fluctuating asymmetry (FA; Livshits & Kobyliansky, 1991; Mather, 1953; Møller & Thornhill, 1998; Polak, 2003; Van Valen, 1962; Zakharov, 1981). FA is characterized by random deviations from perfect symmetry in bilaterally paired traits (e.g., left eye equally likely to be larger or smaller than right eye) that arise when environmental stress and/or genetic factors disrupt development (Møller & Swaddle, 1997; Polak, 2003). In contrast, directional asymmetry (DA) has a consistent bias across a population (e.g., left eye always larger than right eye) and does not reflect developmental instability (Polak, 2003).

Interestingly, human perceptions of facial symmetry are driven largely by FA and are unrelated to DA (Simmons, Rhodes, Peters, & Koehler, 2004).1 Given that humans find symmetric faces attractive (for reviews, see Rhodes, 2006; Rhodes & Simmons, 2007), this focus on facial FA could be evolutionarily adaptive if facial FA covaries negatively with mate quality. Evidence that facial asymmetry is elevated in some chromosomal disorders (Thornhill & Møller, 1997) suggests that it might. In addition, facial asymmetry has been linked with poor health in nonclinical populations, although this finding remains controversial (for a recent review, see Rhodes & Simmons, 2007).

What proximate mechanism could filter out DA and focus perception on asymmetries that may signal developmental instability? A good candidate is perceptual adaptation, which occurs when response properties change in response to changes in the inputs (Clifford & Rhodes, 2005; Clifford et al., 2007; Schwartz, Hsu, & Dayan, 2007). Perceptual adaptation may serve to calibrate responses to the distribution of inputs experienced and would ensure sensitivity to distributional properties of the inputs, such as whether an asymmetric trait in a face is FA or DA.

Numerous studies have reported perceptual adaptation in face perception (MacLin & Webster, 2001; Rhodes, Jeffery, Watson, Clifford, & Nakayama, 2003; Rhodes et al., 2004; Robbins, McKone, & Edwards, 2007; Watson & Clifford, 2003; Webster & MacLin, 1999). In these studies, participants typically view a series of faces to which a consistent distortion has been applied. For example, the internal features may have been "expanded" spherically from a central point, making the faces look as if they were viewed through a fish-eye lens. After the viewer has adapted to these faces, the distortion becomes less noticeable and faces with slightly expanded features look more normal than undistorted faces. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Perceptual Adaptation to Facial Asymmetries
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.