Readers of Chinese Extract Semantic Information from Parafoveal Words

By Yan, Ming; Richter, Eike M. et al. | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, June 2009 | Go to article overview

Readers of Chinese Extract Semantic Information from Parafoveal Words


Yan, Ming, Richter, Eike M., Shu, Hua, Kliegl, Reinhold, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


Evidence for semantic preview benefit (PB) from parafoveal words has been elusive for reading alphabetic scripts such as English. Here we report semantic PB for noncompound characters in Chinese reading with the boundary paradigm. In addition, PBs for orthographic relatedness and, as a numeric trend, for phonological relatedness were obtained. Results are in agreement with other research suggesting that the Chinese writing system is based on a closer association between graphic form and meaning than is alphabetic script. We discuss implications for notions of serial attention shifts and parallel distributed processing of words during reading.

(ProQuest: ... denotes non-USASCII text omitted.)

A key result of research on eye movement in reading is that preview of a word to the right of fixation leads to shorter fixations on this word when it is fixated after the next saccade (Rayner, 1975). This preview benefit (PB) of a target word is established relative to fixation durations on unrelated preview words that are replaced by the target word only after the eye crosses an invisible boundary between them. PB has been demonstrated not only for the identical word, but also for words that are orthographically (Inhoff, 1990; Inhoff & Tousman, 1990; Rayner, 1975) or phonologically (e.g., Pollatsek, Lesch, Morris, & Rayner, 1992) related to the target, or that are predictable from prior sentence context (e.g., Balota, Pollatsek, & Rayner, 1985).

Somewhat surprisingly, there is still no statistically reliable result to show that semantic information is extracted from a parafoveal word during either first fixations or gaze durations in alphabetic writing systems (Rayner, Balota, & Pollatsek, 1986; for a review, see Rayner, White, Kambe, Miller, & Liversedge, 2003). This null result is compatible with the assumption that word recognition in alphabetic languages adheres to the well-known triad of orthographic, phonological, and semantic processing (e.g., Coltheart, Rastle, Perry, Langdon, & Ziegler, 2001), with semantic information becoming available relatively late in the processing chain. For reasons elaborated below, we expected this to be different for Chinese script, and, indeed, we will demonstrate reliable evidence for early semantic information extraction.

Relevant Features of the Chinese Writing System

Chinese text is written in a series of square-shaped characters with the same width, irrespective of their visual complexity. Characters are formed according to a number of principles (Feng, Miller, Shu, & Zhang, 2001): Most characters are compound characters and have two components/radicals (Yin & Rohsenow, 1994), one of which represents the meaning of the character, and the other of which provides a rough clue about its pronunciation. 1 The pronunciation of a certain character cannot be derived directly from its orthography because only about 30% of these characters have the same pronunciation as their phonetic component (Gao, Fan, & Fei, 1993; Zhou & Marslen-Wilson, 1999).

There are also pictographical characters (e.g., the characters ... and ..., which mean horse and wagon, respectively) that originated from ancient drawings by cave dwellers, and indicative characters (e.g., the characters ... and ..., which look like upward and downward arrows and mean top and bottom, respectively) that were formed by analogy or association. In our experiment, we used these visually and structurally simple and relatively common pictographic and indicative characters as target words to achieve independence of orthographic and phonological features and to maximize chances of observing semantic PB. In summary, in comparison with alphabetic languages, Chinese is generally mapped more closely to meaning than to phonology, and this holds especially for the material used in the present experiment.

Perceptual Span in Chinese Reading

In Chinese reading, the perceptual span2 is surprisingly narrow, extending at most up to one character to the left and two to three characters to the right of fixation (Inhoff & Liu, 1997, 1998; C. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Readers of Chinese Extract Semantic Information from Parafoveal Words
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.