Language and Literacy Acquisition through Parental Mediation in American Sign Language

By Bailes, Cynthia Neese; Erting, Carol J. et al. | Sign Language Studies, Summer 2009 | Go to article overview

Language and Literacy Acquisition through Parental Mediation in American Sign Language


Bailes, Cynthia Neese, Erting, Carol J., Erting, Lynne C., Thumann-Prezioso, Carlene, Sign Language Studies


Culturally situated social interactions during everyday activities play a fundamental role in the development of language and cognition in young children (Bruner 1983, 1990; Ochs 1988; Ochs and Schieffelin 1979; Rogoff 1990, 1997, 2003; Vygotsky 1978). ] From the moment they are born, infants are exposed to people speaking around and to them. As they go about the business of everyday life, parents and caregivers, siblings and extended family intuitively engage the newborn in social interaction and expose the infant to the language the child will eventually acquire and use to make sense of the world (Papousek and Papousek 1987). Now consider this same scenario with one change - the newborn is deaf. The newborn sees the lights, movements, colors, and faces in the environment, but the words spoken go unheard. Whether or not the caregivers are aware that their newborn is deaf, the process of language acquisition and subsequent languagemediated cognitive development does not proceed as it would if the infant had full auditory access to the spoken language that its parents know and use every day. Even on the first day of life, this newborn deaf child is steps behind the infant who began hearing the language used in utero (Mehler, Jusczyk, Lambertz, Halsted, Bertoncini, and AmielTison 1988).

More than 90 percent of all deaf babies are born into this unusual linguistic environment (Mitchell and Karchmer 2004). Upon learning their baby is deaf, parents and caregivers often face emotional turmoil, grief, and sometimes denial. They are likely to be at a loss as to how to meet their baby's unique linguistic needs. Seeking guidance, they frequently find conflicting, confusing and even misleading information (see Meadow- Orlans, Mertens, and Sass-Lehrer [2003] and Mertens, Sass-Lehrer, and Scott-Olson [2000]). Decisions about speech and sign language are usually framed as options in "appropriate communication strategies" and learning to "communicate effectively" (ibid., 142) rather than as choices about what they must do to ensure their child's right to unimpeded and timely language acquisition and cognitive development (Grosjean 2001).

Many parents, because they are not fully aware of what is at stake and thus accept the long-standing but erroneous claim that acquiring a signed language will impede speech development, seek resources devoted to supporting spoken language learning. If they decide they want to use a signed language, they soon realize they are unprepared to fully meet their child's immediate linguistic and cognitive needs. Meanwhile, every day that goes by is another day these deaf children live without the opportunity to acquire language because they are not exposed to a natural language that is fully accessible to them through their eyes.

Historically, discussions about the language and literacy acquisition of deaf children have adopted the perspective that most deaf children are language delayed and have centered on a lapse in the development of spoken and written English. This perceived lag is often cited as the culprit for their subsequent lack of academic achievement, particularly in reading and Avriting English. A groAving perspective today is that language delay is not the cause of this achievement lag; rather, evidence suggests that it is the result of a more serious condition - language deprivation (e.g., Kuntze 1998; Nelson 1998). Indeed, when he first observed and theorized about deaf children in Russia, Vygotsky held that signed languages interfere Avith spoken language development. However, after ten years of research, he concluded that signed languages were necessary requisites for all aspects of the deaf child's development. Moreover, he came to realize that, for deaf children, spoken language was an inadequate tool for acquiring cultural experience and participating in social life and that efforts spent primarily in the pursuit of spoken language learning were not only misdirected but also were often carried out at the expense of cognitive and other critical areas of the deaf child's development (Vygotsky 1993 ; Zaitseva, Pursglove, and Gregory 1999). …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Language and Literacy Acquisition through Parental Mediation in American Sign Language
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.