Typographic Cueing on Screen

By Dyson, Mary C.; Gregory, Judy | Visible Language, January 1, 2002 | Go to article overview

Typographic Cueing on Screen


Dyson, Mary C., Gregory, Judy, Visible Language


The effects of typographic cues, such as bold, underline, italic, capitals have been studied in print. The indications are that typographic cueing can improve the recall of material and this is most evident if recall immediately follows reading. This study investigates whether cueing on screen facilitates recall and introduces factors that have been explored when cueing content in printed material. A series of documents was read on screen followed by a set of multiple choice questions, which covered a range of question types. Cued material was either a phrase or sentence in red type, and these related to either main facts or incidental details. A control condition contained no red. Instruction regarding cueing was also varied.

We found a difference in overall recall between the cueing conditions, but no significant difference between the experimental conditions and the control. The difference was attributable to better recall of cued phrases than cued sentences. However, this difference was found only for incidental material These results suggest that cueing a whole sentence containing detail can hinder overall recall, but cueing the specific detail is helpful.

Looking at answers to particular questions, we found an interaction between the instruction regarding the use of red and the type of question. For most questions performance was better without instruction regarding red type, but this was not the case for some questions. Telling participants that red highlights the answers to some of the questions helped them locate where an item came within the text, if red was used.

Overall, these findings provide no evidence that cueing is generally useful, despite the close relationship between some cued material and questions and explicit information on this relationship. Mixing questions on cued and non-cued material may remove any possible benefits of cueing.

INTRODUCTION

Typographic cueing provides a non-verbal way of guiding readers and focusing attention. By using typographic techniques (such as bold, underline, italics or color) to cue material, information designers can help readers to focus on material that requires the most attention. Most existing work has looked at the application of cueing to printed material, whereas this study explores typographic cueing on screen.

Glynn (1978) suggests that one of the assumptions underlying typographic cueing is that it works because of an isolation effect; by using cueing to set apart some information, lhe cued information is more likely to be noticed by readers.This is also referred to as making material perceptually more salient (Ausubel, Novak and Hanasian, 1978). But while there is wide agreement on why typographic cueing may be useful for readers, there are few guidelines discussing what cues are appropriate and little research to suggest how cues are best used (Beck, 1991). The general consensus emerging from the literature is that typographic cueing can improve the recall of cued material. Foster (1979), in his overview of the cueing literature, argues that there is considerable evidence that typographic cueing can improve recall. Improved recall is demonstrated most frequently if it is tested immediately after reading (193). Foster's conclusion is echoed by Beck (1984, 1991) who notes that cueing generally helps readers to recall the cued information.

While typographic cueing may generally improve the recall of cued material some research suggests that excessive cueing may reverse any positive effects. Excessive cueing may make it difficult for readers to focus on important information.

Marks (1966) explored the quantity of material that should be cued through a study of children's reading of simple instructions. Marks found that cueing key words (by using bold or larger type) tended to improve performance, but cueing the entire set of instructions (through underlining) was detrimental to performance. Although Marks' study suggests that cueing key words rather than entire passages may improve readers' performance, it is not possible to draw this conclusion from the study because of the different cueing devices used. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Typographic Cueing on Screen
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.