When the Going Gets Tough the Beautiful Get Going: Aesthetic Appeal Facilitates Task Performance

By Reppa, Irene; McDougall, Siné | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, October 2015 | Go to article overview

When the Going Gets Tough the Beautiful Get Going: Aesthetic Appeal Facilitates Task Performance


Reppa, Irene, McDougall, Siné, Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


Published online: 17 January 2015

© The Author(s) 2014. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com

Abstract The current studies examined the effect of aesthetic appeal on performance. According to one hypothesis, appeal would lead to overall decrements or enhancements in performance [e.g. Sonderegger & Sauer, (Applied Ergonomics, 41, 403-410, 2010)]. Alternatively, appeal might influence performance only in problem situations, such as when the task is difficult [e.g. Norman, (2004)]. The predictions of these hypotheses were examined in the context of an icon search-and-localisation task. Icons were used because they are well-defined stimuli and pervasive to modern everyday life. When search was made difficult using visually complex stimuli (Experiment 1), or abstract and unfamiliar stimuli (Experiment 2), icons that were appealing were found more quickly than their unappealing counterparts. These findings show that in a low-level visual processing task, with demand characteristics related to appeal eliminated, appeal can influence performance, especially under duress.

Keywords Attention . Human factors . Visual perception

Introduction

Can visual aesthetic appeal influence task performance? This is an intriguing question with potentially far-reaching practical and theoretical implications (e.g. Norman, 2004). The interest in the relationship between aesthetic appeal and performance is not new. Early studies on the relationship between aesthetic appeal and task performance revealed strong correlations between judgments of aesthetic appeal and judgments of how easy-to-use a product appears to be (e.g. Jordan, 1998; Kurosu & Kashimura, 1995; Lingaard & Dudek, 2003; Tractinsky, Katz, & Ikar, 2000; Tractinsky, 2004; Wiedenbeck, 1999; see Hassenzahl & Monk, 2010, for review). Experimental study of whether aesthetic appeal might influence actual task performance has only recently started to make headway (e.g. Thüring & Mahlke, 2007; Moshagen, Musch, & Göritz, 2009; Sauer & Sonderegger, 2009; 2011; Sonderegger & Sauer, 2010).

The handful of studies examining whether visual aesthetic appeal might influence performance has yielded mixed support for this notion. Some studies have found no effect of stimulus appeal on task performance (e.g. Hartmann, Sutcliffe, & De Angeli, 2007; Thüring & Mahlke, 2007; Tractinsky et al., 2000), while others have found conflicting results (e.g. Moshagen et al., 2009; Sonderegger & Sauer, 2009). Positive evidence suggests that appealing stimuli can increase performance efficiency (e.g. Moshagen et al., 2009; Sonderegger & Sauer, 2010) and perseverance with the task (e.g. Nakarada-Kordich & Lobb, 2005). In contrast, decreased performance efficiency for appealing stimuli has sometimes been reported (e.g. Ben-Bassat et al., 2006; Meyer, Shinar, & Leiser, 1997; Tufte, 1983; Sauer & Sonderegger, 2009; 2011).

Conflicting findings may be due to the fact that aesthetic appeal1 is a multi-dimensional construct influenced by a number of factors, including colour (e.g. Palmer, Schloss, & Sammartino, 2013), visual complexity (e.g., Eisenman, 1967; Jacobsen & Höfel, 2002; Martindale, Moore, & West, 1988), symmetry and balance (e.g. Jacobsen & Höfel, 2002; Palmer & Griscom, 2013), meaningfulness (e.g. Russell, 2003; Leder, Carbon, & Ripsas, 2006), familiarity (e.g. Bornstein, 1989; Lindgaard, Fernandes, Dudek, & Brown, 2006; Reber, Schwarz, & Winkielman, 2004;Zajonc,1968, 1998, 2000), and concreteness (e.g. Kawabata & Zeki, 2004; Vartanian & Goel, 2004), to name a few. Such factors may be confounding variables underlying the conflicting evidence regarding whether aesthetic appeal influences performance. For instance, sometimes manipulations of appeal have affected the visual complexity of the artifact under investigation (e.g. computerized mobile phone: Sonderegger & Sauer, 2010; computerised phone-book: Ben-Bassat et al. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA 8, MLA 7, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Note: primary sources have slightly different requirements for citation. Please see these guidelines for more information.

Cited article

When the Going Gets Tough the Beautiful Get Going: Aesthetic Appeal Facilitates Task Performance
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen
Items saved from this article
  • Highlights & Notes
  • Citations
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA 8, MLA 7, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Search by... Author
    Show... All Results Primary Sources Peer-reviewed

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.