Does Overconfidence Lead to Poor Decisions? A Comparison of Decision Making and Judgment under Uncertainty

By Bukszar, Ed | Journal of Business and Management, Winter 2003 | Go to article overview

Does Overconfidence Lead to Poor Decisions? A Comparison of Decision Making and Judgment under Uncertainty


Bukszar, Ed, Journal of Business and Management


Two within-subject studies of business executives indicate that overconfidence in judgment is reduced when actual decisions are made. Subjects projected quarterly earnings for 50 firms based on reported earnings from the previous 12 quarters. They stated their confidence in forecasts, were given a $10 allocation and were allowed to invest in any, all or none of their forecasts. Subjects chose to act on a relatively small portion of their forecasts but were more accurate and better calibrated when making investment decisions than when making forecasts. The relatively more accurate subjects made more investment decisions and riskier decisions than the relatively less accurate subjects. This suggests that subjects had a sense of whether their knowledge was appropriate for decision making, and acted accordingly. Improved calibration for decision making (investments) compared to judgments (forecasts) appears to have been the result of an additional evaluation stage which occurred between making a forecast and acting upon it. Most subjects tended to 'think twice' before acting, which may have lead, to more thorough information processing and improved calibration.

Findings in the literature on overconfidence are wide-ranging and robust (Fischhoff, 1982; Lichtenstein, Fischhoff, & Phillips, 1982).1 Poorly calibrated decision makers are thought to make ill-advised choices based on sub-optimal information searches. Generalizing somewhat, decision makers tend to be overconfident for difficult problems and underconfident for easy problems (Lichtenstein & Fischhoff, 1977). Findings are not unequivocal with respect to experts. Some show significant overconfidence while others show reasonably well calibrated experts. Characteristics of the decision tasks may account for the differences (Keren, 1991 ). Weather forecasters, accountants and loan officers can be trained to be calibrated due to the clarity and timeliness of feedback while physicians, psychologists and strategists are more prone to overconfidence due to the perceived uniqueness of the circumstances at hand and the difficulty of learning from feedback under these circumstances (Christensen-Szalanski & Busheyhead, 1981; Hogarth, 1981; Oskamp, 1965; Russo & Schoemaker, 1992; Tomassini, Solomon, Romney, & Krogstad, 1982).

Relatively unaddressed in this research is the question of whether there is a difference between overconfidence with respect to judgments and overconfidence in decisions. Most studies of overconfidence require subjects to evaluate information and state their confidence level in judgments they would make from the information. The findings of overconfidence in the reported judgments is taken as an indication that overconfidence would also be evident in decision making, that is, that overconfidence in judgment leads to ill-advised action. However, in many real-world circumstances, people are not required to act on their judgments. Action implies consequences for which decision makers are often held accountable. If they believe their knowledge is limited, individuals may restrict their decision making, that is, the items upon which they are prepared to act, to a subset of their judgments. (Clearly, in some instances, it is a decision to not act upon an item, particularly when non-action implies consequences. However, in general, I mean to restrict the category 'decision' to actions based upon judgments.)

For example, most of us have expressed an opinion rather strongly only to recant somewhat when asked to back it up with action, in the form of a bet or an investment. Timidity with respect to actual decisions is likely the result of loss aversion as decision makers have been shown to weigh losses at least twice as much as gains (Kahneman & Lovallo, 1993; Kahneman & Tversky, 1979). Faced with the possibility of a loss, decision makers 'think twice' before acting, resulting in somewhat cautious behavior. Overall, the result may be a "conjunction of biases" (Kahneman & Lovallo, 1993, p. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Does Overconfidence Lead to Poor Decisions? A Comparison of Decision Making and Judgment under Uncertainty
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Author Advanced search

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.