Skinner's Box or Pandora's Box: Ten Landmark Psychological Studies on the Couch

By Butler, Katy | Psychotherapy Networker, July/August 2004 | Go to article overview

Skinner's Box or Pandora's Box: Ten Landmark Psychological Studies on the Couch


Butler, Katy, Psychotherapy Networker


Skinner's Box or Pandora's Box?

Ten landmark psychological studies on the couch

BY Katy Butler

In Opening Skinner's Box --the fascinating, flawed, and controversial psychology book published by W.W. Norton in March--author Lauren Slater revisits one of the 20th century's most influential psychology experiments. It was 1961, a decade before My Lai and four decades before Abu Ghraib. In New Haven, Connecticut, a 27-year-old social psychologist named Stanley Milgram set out to explain why ordinary human beings in Nazi Germany had inflicted cruelty on innocent strangers. He was skeptical of the psychoanalytically influenced theories of the day that abusive German childraising practices had created an "authoritarian personality" willing to scapegoat others as an outlet for its own disowned aggression. Far more significant, he thought, was the shaping power of the social situations in which German adults had found themselves.

So Milgram, an assistant professor at Yale, perpetrated what Slater calls "one of psychology's grandest and most horrible hoaxes." Pretending to be studying the effects of punishment on learning in a lab in the basement of Yale's Linsley-Chittenden Hall, he instructed unwitting subjects to give increasingly intense "shocks" to a man who was actually an actor and an accomplice. When ordered to do so, 65 percent of the volunteers pulled levers that said "Danger, Extreme Shock: 340 Volts" even while the supposed victim writhed in feigned agony, screamed, begged for mercy, and fell into deathly silence. They did it, apparently, because a man in a white coat told them to. So much for theories about German childraising. If a hypothetically evil U.S. government tried to set up death camps, Milgram wrote to his backers at the National Science Foundation, New Haven alone could produce a full complement of "moral imbeciles" willing to man them.

Technically, Milgram's was a study of the effect of obedience and social influence on human behavior. But as Slater asks in Opening Skinner's Box, what about the story behind the story? Should we pay no attention to the man behind the curtain--to Milgram himself, raised in the South Bronx hearing the news of the Nazi death camps on the family radio? Should we ignore the social climate of his times, which denied him tenure at Yale because of outrage about the ethics of his experiment? Were his results simply objective pieces of data, the latest bricks in the growing edifice of a truly scientific psychology? Or does Heisenberg's Uncertainty Principle (which holds that the presence of an observer changes the thing observed) obtain in the world of psychology as well as in quantum mechanics?

Working in an agnostic century horrified by the trench warfare of World War I and the collective madness of the Holocaust, Milgram had implictly raised large, messy questions that reverberated beyond the narrow confines of his basement lab, questions that resonate to this day: What's free will, and do we have it? What makes us capable of being cruel, or of witnessing cruelty as passive bystanders? What primarily propels us--personality, social situations, drives, neurobiology, upbringing, or moral or religious teachings--toward good or evil? Are these questions reducible to an experimental design? And is it ethical--or unjustifiably cruel--to hoax innocent volunteers into giving others "shocks" in the interest of advancing science?

Little of this back-story could be gleaned from the dry scientific language in which this influential experiment was originally published. Now Slater, a memoirist and counseling psychologist, has retold it, and nine other experiments like it, in compelling, novelistic terms. She imagines Milgram as "Part imp, a little Jewish leprechaun, his science soaked in joke" and writes that in another experiment he pointed up at an empty sky and timed how long it took to gather a crowd looking up at nothing. His obedience experiment inspired public loathing: Bruno Bettelheim called him "vile" and child psychologist Diana Baumrind attacked him for traumatizing human subjects. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Skinner's Box or Pandora's Box: Ten Landmark Psychological Studies on the Couch
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.