Primacy, Recency, and Suffix Effects in Auditory Short-Term Memory for Pure Tones: Evidence from a Probe Recognition Paradigm

By Mondor, Todd A.; Morin, Simone R. | Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, September 2004 | Go to article overview

Primacy, Recency, and Suffix Effects in Auditory Short-Term Memory for Pure Tones: Evidence from a Probe Recognition Paradigm


Mondor, Todd A., Morin, Simone R., Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology


Abstract

The present study was designed to explore serial position and suffix effects in the short-term retention of nonverbal sounds. In contrast with previous studies of these effects, a probe recognition paradigm was used to minimize the possibility that participants would use a verbal labelling strategy. On each trial, participants heard a memory set consisting of three pure tones, followed five seconds later by a probe tone. Participants were required to indicate whether or not the probe tone had been a member of the memory set. On most trials, a suffix sound was presented 1 second following the third sound in the memory set. Results revealed that tones presented in the first and last positions of the memory set were recognized more accurately than were tones presented in the middle position. Furthermore, recognition of sounds presented in the last position was compromised when the memory set was followed by a postlist suffix of similar pitch, spectral composition, and spatial location.

Empirical investigations of the nature of short-term memory have typically required participants to remember items that could be easily verbally labelled, such as letters (Crowder, 1968; Jolicoeur & Dell-Acqua, 1998; Murray, 1968), digits (e.g., Moray & Jordan, 1966; Salame & Baddeley, 1982; Wickelgren, 1965), words (e.g., Copeland & Radvansky, 2001; Loess, 1967), and pictures of common objects (e.g., Coltheart, 1999; Yuille & Fox, 1973). However, as Greene (1988) has argued, such an approach to developing an understanding of short-term memory may be limited because memory for such stimuli "could reflect more general aspects of memory, and be influenced by mechanisms specific to speech processing" (p. 247). For this reason, explorations of short-term memory using stimuli that are difficult or impossible to label verbally are important because they may provide a more thorough understanding of the nature of short-term memory. Empirical investigation of short-term memoiy for nonverbal visual material has generally shown better memory for the first few items in a memory set (this is typically referred to as a primacy effect) for stimuli such as snowflake pictures (Neath & Knoedler, 1994), and kaleidoscope patterns (Wright, 1994; Wright et al., 1990). Because these types of stimuli cannot be easily labelled verbally, the evidence of primacy obtained for them suggests that their retention is not dependent on verbal labelling. This evidence stands in sharp contrast to the suggestions of some theorists that primacy effects are necessarily dependent on verbal processing strategies (e.g., Rundus & Atkinson, 1970).

A variety of studies have been directed toward determining whether factors known to affect retention of verbal material hold also for short-term memory of nonverbal sounds. Thus, for example, it appears clear that memory for pitch decays steadily over time (e.g., Egan, Schulman, & Greenberg, 1961; Eriksen & Johnson, 1964; Harris, 1952; Massaro, 1972b; Moss, Myers, & Filmore, 1970; Pollack, 1964; but see Cowan, Saults, & Nugent, 2001, for a desccnting view). The possibility that serial position effects may arise in the short-term retention of nonverbal sounds has been the subject of a number of studies. In several of these experiments, a serial recall procedure has been used wherein listeners were required to list the sounds that they had heard in the order in which they were presented (e.g., Foreit, 1976; Rowe, 1974; Surprenant, Pitt, & Crowder, 1993). The serial position effects reported in these studies have varied somewhat with some investigators reporting only primacy (Foreit; Surprenant et al., 1993) and others reporting both primacy and recency (Greene & Samuel, 1986; Roberts, 1986; Rowe; Rowe & Rowe; 1976). Because written serial recall was required in all of these studies, however, it is possible that the serial position effects that were obtained may have been driven by retention of the verbal labels or written symbols that were to be recalled rather than by memory for the acoustic characteristics of the sounds themselves. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

Primacy, Recency, and Suffix Effects in Auditory Short-Term Memory for Pure Tones: Evidence from a Probe Recognition Paradigm
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.