On the Mental Representation of Negative Numbers: Context-Dependent SNARC Effects with Comparative Judgments

By Shaki, Samuel; Petrusic, William M. | Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, October 2005 | Go to article overview

On the Mental Representation of Negative Numbers: Context-Dependent SNARC Effects with Comparative Judgments


Shaki, Samuel, Petrusic, William M., Psychonomic Bulletin & Review


In one condition, positive and negative number pairs were compared in separate blocks of trials. In another condition, the positive and the negative number pairs were intermixed. In the intermixed condition, comparisons involving negative numbers were faster with the left hand than with the right, and comparisons were faster with the right hand than with the left hand with the positive numbers; that is, a spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect was obtained, in which the mental number line was extended leftward with the negative numbers. On the other hand, in the blocked condition, a reverse SNARC effect was obtained with the negative numbers; that is, negative number pairs have the same underlying spatial representation as the positive numbers in this context. Nongraded semantic congruity effects, obtained in both the blocked and the intermixed conditions, are consistent with the idea that magnitude information is extracted prior to the generation of discrete semantic codes.

At the outset, in the teaching of number concepts to elementary school children, numbers are depicted in spatial terms. For example, Del Grande, Jones, Lowe, and Morrow (1974), in their text for the sixth grade, introduce integers in terms of walks. As such, integers represent both length and direction. They write the following:

Here is Larry on a sidewalk. The sidewalk is marked off at regular intervals. If Larry walks 3 spaces to the right, we call it a +3 walk (positive 3). If he walks 4 spaces to the left, we call it a -4 walk (negative 4). (p. 60)

Although the concept of integers as points on the number line is the basis for their mathematical representation, is there evidence for a mental representation of integers in terms of length and direction-that is, a mental number line!

Indeed, two lines of evidence, taken together, suggest that the mental representation of numbers is in terms of extents. First, following on Moyer and Landauer's (1967) seminal work, a split, or distance, effect is now a hallmark of comparative judgments with numbers (and symbolic comparison generally): The larger the difference between the numbers compared, the shorter the response time (RT). second, depending on the range of numbers used, a magnitude effect can be obtained: With split held constant at, say, one, as the numbers compared become larger, RTs increase, reflecting a Weber-law-like property. The distance effect and the magnitude effect, taken together, have led researchers (e.g., Dehaene, 1992) to postulate a compressive analogue mental representation for numbers; that is, numbers are represented in terms of length.

The most striking and influential demonstration of a spatial basis for the mental representation of a number line, with numbers arranged from left to right, was obtained by Dehaene, Bossini, and Giraux (1993). Dehaene et al. required their participants to classify the digits from zero to nine as odd or even (i.e., to make parity judgments). They found that parity judgments with the relatively small numbers in the set were faster with the left hand than with the right hand. However, when the numbers were relatively large, the participants responded more quickly with their right hand. The authors argued that since large numbers are represented to the right on the number line, they preferentially elicit a rightward response. Similarly, since small numbers are represented to the left on the number line, they become more readily associated with the left hand than with the right hand. Capturing the association between the spatial component of the mental representation of numbers and the hand of the response, Dehaene et al. labeled their finding as the spatial numerical association of response codes (SNARC) effect.

A clear extension of the SNARC effect (and the teaching of arithmetic) suggests that the number line extends leftward to include the negative numbers (see Fischer, 2003a). On the other hand, it is possible that negative numbers do not have a psychological reality. …

The rest of this article is only available to active members of Questia

Already a member? Log in now.

Notes for this article

Add a new note
If you are trying to select text to create highlights or citations, remember that you must now click or tap on the first word, and then click or tap on the last word.
One moment ...
Default project is now your active project.
Project items

Items saved from this article

This article has been saved
Highlights (0)
Some of your highlights are legacy items.

Highlights saved before July 30, 2012 will not be displayed on their respective source pages.

You can easily re-create the highlights by opening the book page or article, selecting the text, and clicking “Highlight.”

Citations (0)
Some of your citations are legacy items.

Any citation created before July 30, 2012 will labeled as a “Cited page.” New citations will be saved as cited passages, pages or articles.

We also added the ability to view new citations from your projects or the book or article where you created them.

Notes (0)
Bookmarks (0)

You have no saved items from this article

Project items include:
  • Saved book/article
  • Highlights
  • Quotes/citations
  • Notes
  • Bookmarks
Notes
Cite this article

Cited article

Style
Citations are available only to our active members.
Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

(Einhorn, 1992, p. 25)

(Einhorn 25)

1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

Cited article

On the Mental Representation of Negative Numbers: Context-Dependent SNARC Effects with Comparative Judgments
Settings

Settings

Typeface
Text size Smaller Larger Reset View mode
Search within

Search within this article

Look up

Look up a word

  • Dictionary
  • Thesaurus
Please submit a word or phrase above.
Print this page

Print this page

Why can't I print more than one page at a time?

Help
Full screen

matching results for page

    Questia reader help

    How to highlight and cite specific passages

    1. Click or tap the first word you want to select.
    2. Click or tap the last word you want to select, and you’ll see everything in between get selected.
    3. You’ll then get a menu of options like creating a highlight or a citation from that passage of text.

    OK, got it!

    Cited passage

    Style
    Citations are available only to our active members.
    Buy instant access to cite pages or passages in MLA, APA and Chicago citation styles.

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn, 1992, p. 25).

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences." (Einhorn 25)

    "Portraying himself as an honest, ordinary person helped Lincoln identify with his audiences."1

    1. Lois J. Einhorn, Abraham Lincoln, the Orator: Penetrating the Lincoln Legend (Westport, CT: Greenwood Press, 1992), 25, http://www.questia.com/read/27419298.

    Cited passage

    Thanks for trying Questia!

    Please continue trying out our research tools, but please note, full functionality is available only to our active members.

    Your work will be lost once you leave this Web page.

    Buy instant access to save your work.

    Already a member? Log in now.

    Oops!

    An unknown error has occurred. Please click the button below to reload the page. If the problem persists, please try again in a little while.